Controlled Processing (Psychology): Definition and Examples

Controlled Processing (Psychology): Definition and ExamplesReviewed by Chris Drew (PhD)

This article was peer-reviewed and edited by Chris Drew (PhD). The review process on Helpful Professor involves having a PhD level expert fact check, edit, and contribute to articles. Reviewers ensure all content reflects expert academic consensus and is backed up with reference to academic studies. Dr. Drew has published over 20 academic articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education and holds a PhD in Education from ACU.

controlled processing examples and definition, explained below

Controlled processing is the term used for cognitive activities that are intentionally activated and require conscious awareness. To perform novel or difficult tasks involves controlled processing.

For example, individuals are using controlled processing when engaged in problem-solving.First, several possible solutions must be generated. Then, a thorough analysis of factors that may facilitate or hinder the success of that solution is initiated.

This might involve accessing previous experiences from memory or recalling information read weeks or months prior.

Additionally, the pros and cons of each solution must be considered, which requires thinking about future circumstances and trying to anticipate potential obstacles.

Each one of these elements of problem-solving is a function of controlled mental activity that requires a great deal of concentration and effort.

In fact, controlled processing is sometimes referred to as effortful processing.

Controlled Processing vs. Automatic Processing

There are four fundamental differences between controlled and automatic processing.

1. Active vs Passive

Controlled processing is an active cognitive process whereas automatic processing is passive.

This means that there is at least a minimal amount of mental effort involved in controlled processes. In some cases, there may be substantial mental effort required.

However, automatic processes involve almost zero mental effort. In fact, there are several forms of automatic processing that do not require any mental effort whatsoever.

2. Cognitive Capacity

The second difference has to do with what is referred to as cognitive capacity.

At any given time, while engaged in a mental activity, each person is utilizing a portion of their cognitive capacity.

Some tasks are so difficult that they require 100% of an individual’s cognitive capacity to perform.

Controlled processes can absorb maximum cognitive capacity. Automatic processing absorbs zero, or near zero cognitive capacity.

For example, think of a person driving to an unfamiliar destination in a neighborhood. As they are driving along slowly and trying to read the street signs, they will often turn down the volume on the radio or ask their passengers to be quiet.

This is because the task uses a lot of cognitive capacity and the person cannot complete the task if being distracted.

3. Conscious Control of Attention

The third difference has to do with the conscious control of attention.

Controlled processing involves the individual consciously directing their attention to various aspects of the situation.

Automatic processing does not require conscious control of attention. Many tasks can be performed unconsciously, with absolutely zero conscious control.

4. Speed

Finally, automatic processes are fast. This is because they absorb very little cognitive capacity.

However, controlled processes are slower.

The more difficult or novel the task, the slower the controlled processing becomes in order to perform the task.

Relationship Between Controlled and Automatic Processing

Although usually discussed as being a dichotomy, controlled and automatic processing exists on a continuum (Hartlage et al., 1993).  

A task which began requiring controlled processing may become automatic with repetition.

That is, a task which began as needing a great deal of attentional control and absorbing a lot of cognitive capacity, can eventually become automatic.

For example, when a child is first learning to read a simple word such as “cat,” they have to think about how to pronounce each letter. This first step in reading is performed sequentially, and slowly.

But with practice, the child will no longer have to access their long-term memory every time to recall how to pronounce each one of those letters.

It happens automatically. They see the word and instantly pronounce it correctly.

The task no longer requires a lot of conscious effort and it absorbs very little cognitive capacity. What was once a controlled process has become automatic.

Although controlled processing requires cognitive capacity and attentional resources,

“the costs of this capacity limitation are balanced by the benefits deriving from the ease with which such processes may be set up, altered, and applied in novel situations for which automatic sequences have never been learned” (Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977, p. 3).

Schneider (1985, 2016) proposed that moving from controlled to automatic processing when a person conducts a search through occurs in four phases:

  • Phase 1: Phase 1 is purely a function of controlled processing.
  • Phase 2: As repetition increases, Phase 2 involves both controlled and automatic processing co-occurring.
  • Phase 3: Phase 3 still involves controlled processing, but it is done so in order to guide automatic processing.
  • Phase 4: Eventually, the entire task becomes a function of automatic processing. In this last phase, other tasks that require attentional demands and capacity can be activated.

Origins of Controlled Processing

Schneider and Chein (2003) point out that the notion of two types of processing, automatic and controlled, was first suggested by James in 1890 in his famous book, The Principles of Psychology. 

Several decades later, Kahneman (1973) proposed a capacity model of attention which suggested that there is limited energy available to devote to mental activities.

Kahneman identified three elements:

“…the completion of a mental activity requires… “effort,” “capacity,” or “attention.”” (p. 9).

More specifically related to the notion of dual types of cognitive processing, Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) identified the specific characteristics of controlled processing in memory tasks.

From that early publication, Shiffrin and his students continued this line of research for the next 30 years (Schneider & Chein, 2003).

Controlled Processing Examples

  • Learning How to Ride a Bike: In the beginning stages of learning how to ride a bike, the novice needs to exert maximum concentration on all aspects of riding. They have to think about steering, pedaling, keeping their balance, and monitoring the environment for obstacles. 
  • Playing Chess: Every move in chess involves analyzing dozens of play sequences far into the future. To make matters even more challenging, the advantages and risks of each move must also be carefully considered.  
  • Participating in a Classroom Debate: Having a debate with a classmate requires a lot of cognitive processing. The points being made by the other participant have to be quickly analyzed for weaknesses, while at the same time, one must be able to generate a potent counterargument
  • Writing a Scientific Paper: When writing a scientific paper, it is usually necessary to refer back to previous studies, identify key points made, and then incorporate those ideas into what is being currently written. Those ideas might have to also be critiqued, used for making additional points, or for creating context for the current discussion.   
  • Preparing a Speech: Preparing for the delivery of an important speech can require a lot of time spent researching the topic, identifying key points and supporting facts. It is best to memorize the speech so one is not constantly looking down at a sheet of paper and reading from a script. All of that takes considerable control over one’s cognitive processing.     
  • Learning How to Use New Software: The first few weeks trying to learn how to use new software can be very frustrating because it requires so much cognitive effort. Searching for the right icon on the menu that will activate the function you need takes time and usually results in a few mistakes before finding the right one. However, after a few weeks, its easy to fly through the steps needed, almost without even thinking.  
  • Playing the Piano: When first learning how to play the piano, the novice has to concentrate fully on each and every keystroke. The fingers move slowly and it requires a lot of concentration. With time and practice, eventually a song can be performed so easily that it can be played automatically.  
  • Designing a Research Study: When scientists design a research study, they have to consider a lot of issues. How much time will the study take? What resources will they need? What kind of training will the research assistants need? Once the data are collected, which specific statistical analyses are most appropriate? Answering all of those questions will absorb a lot of cognitive capacity and require sustained conscious control of attention.
  • Creating a Concept Map: Creating a concept map involves searching through long-term memory to find definitions and facts, and then examining how that information is related to other concepts. Those comparisons and analyses all involve controlled processing.
  • Implementing an Action Plan: School administrators have just discovered that one of their students has been diagnosed with a contagious disease. They must immediately implement an action plan that takes into account a large number of factors. Some aspects of the plan will need to be modified and various roles and tasks will need to be delegated quickly and efficiently.

Applications of Controlled Processing

1. Effortful Processing Impairments in Depression

Cohen et al. (2001) compared depressed with non-depressed individuals’ performance on various types of tasks.

“Results indicate that patients with major affective disorders show significant attentional impairments on most measures of effortful attention, and the magnitude of these impairments increases as the effortful demands of the task increase” (p. 385).

Other research has found that depressed patients have difficulty planning, initiating tasks, and problem-solving (Martin et al, 1981; Elderkin-Thompson et al., 2006).

Several other studies suggest that individuals with major affective disorders have impaired attentional control and executive functioning (Weingartner, 1981; Cohen, 2013).

These impairments lead to reduced emotional regulation, can lead to impulsive behavior, and disrupt interpersonal relations.

McClintock et al. (2010) suggest that difficulties in executive functions are related to frontal cortical impairments in the cerebral cortex (Kaiser et al., 2003).

2. In Advertising

Controlled and automatic processing of advertisements influence the effectiveness of any given commercial.

For example, some types of commercials contain a lot of factual information regarding a product or service. However, other commercials contain celebrity endorsements or attractive models.

According to the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) of persuasion, each type of commercial will be processed through either a central or peripheral route (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986).

When the viewer knows a lot about the product, issue, or the message is highly self-relevant, then the ELM proposes that the viewer will engage in “careful and thoughtful consideration of the true merits of the information presented” (p. 125).

This is the central route and involves controlled processing of the information presented in the ad.

In contrast, for less personally relevant products or issues in which the viewer knows less, they are more persuaded by the attractiveness or status of the source.

This is the peripheral route, which involves a more automatic process that relies on affective components of the ad.

Since its original proposal in the mid-1980s, the model has been researched extensively and proven to be a highly valuable framework for understanding consumer behavior, public service announcements, prejudice, and attitudes on political issues (Petty & Briñol, 2011).

3. In Combatting Fake News

The spread of misinformation online, commonly referred to as fake news, has raised concern among behavioral scientists and political commentators (Allen et al., 2020).

Fake news can lead to increased political polarization, decreased trust in public institutions, and undermines democracy (Persily, 2017; Tucker et al., 2018).

Eveland’s (2001) cognitive mediation model proposes that cognitive processing of news must occur in order for it to affect an individual’s political views or behavior.

Research by Shahin et al. (2021) support this contention. Their research found that elaborating on news content (i.e., controlled processing), increases online political involvement. 

de Zúñiga et al. (2023) point out that increased elaboration may result in the rejection of fake news. When an individual encounters fake news they may engage in an analysis of the message content (i.e., controlled processing). This enhanced analysis will help them identify the misleading elements of the message.

Their survey results found that “Individuals reporting greater elaboration in response to news were more disposed toward corrective actions, suggesting that news elaboration may serve as a buffer against fake news spread” (p. 3444).

These studies, combined with other research (Pennycook & Rand, 2019; 2020), demonstrate that the failure to reject fake news may be the result of not engaging in effortful (i.e., controlled) processing of message content.

Conclusion

Controlled processing is the term used to describe a mental activity which requires cognitive effort, conscious control of attention, and absorbs cognitive capacity.

When first encountering a difficult task, completing that task is a function of controlled processing. Fortunately, with repetition, most controlled processes can become automatic.

This means that the once difficult tack can be performed with very little cognitive effort, capacity, or attentional focus.

Research has demonstrated that controlled processing can be a challenge for depressed individuals, which may lead to impaired planning and problem-solving.

Because controlled processing is necessary for emotional regulation, depressed individuals may also have difficulty with impulse control or maintaining healthy social relations.

Controlled processing also plays a role in the persuasiveness of message appeals in commercials, political ads, and fake news.

References

Allen, J., Howland, B., Mobius, M., Rothschild, D., & Watts, D. J. (2020). Evaluating the fake news problem at the scale of the information ecosystem. Science Advances, 6(14). doi: https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aay3539

Amazeen, M. A., Thorson, E., Muddiman, A., & Graves, L. (2018). Correcting political and consumer misperceptions: The effectiveness and effects of rating scale versus contextual correction formats. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 95(1), 28–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699016678186

Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

Cohen, R., Lohr, I., Paul, R., & Boland, R. (2001). Impairments of attention and effort among patients with major affective disorders. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 13(3), 385-395.

Cohen, R. A. (2013). The neuropsychology of attention. Springer Science & Business Media.

de Zúñiga, H. G., Goyanes, M., & Skurka, C. (2023). Understanding fake news corrective action: A mixed-method approach. International Journal of Communication, 17, 23.

Elderkin-Thompson, V., Mintz, J., Haroon, E., Lavretsky, H., & Kumar, A. (2006). Executive dysfunction and memory in older patients with major and minor depression. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 669-676. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.021

Eveland, W. P., Jr. (2001). The cognitive mediation model of learning from the news: Evidence from nonelection, off-year election, and presidential election contexts. Communication Research, 28(5), 571–601. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/009365001028005001

Hammar, Å., & Årdal, G. (2012). Effortful information processing in patients with major depression—A 10-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Research, 198(3), 420-423. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.020

Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vázquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 247. doi: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.247

Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 356. doi: https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-3445.108.3.356

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort (Vol. 1063, pp. 218-226). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Kaiser, S., Unger, J., Kiefer, M., Markela, J., Mundt, C., & Weisbrod, M. (2003). Executive control deficit in depression: Event-related potentials in a go/no go task. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 122, 169 –184.

Martin, D. J., Oren, Z., & Boone, K. (1991). Major depressives’ and dysthymics’ performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47(5), 684-690.

McClintock, S. M., Husain, M. M., Greer, T. L., & Cullum, C. M. (2010). Association between depression severity and neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder: a review and synthesis. Neuropsychology, 24(1), 9.

Merrin, E. L. (1985). Motor and sighting dominance in schizophrenia and affective disorder: Evidence for right-hand grip strength prominence in paranoid schizophrenia and bipolar illness. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 146(5), 539-544.

Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019). Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, 188, 39–50.

Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. Journal of Personality, 88(2), 185–200.

Persily, N. (2017). The 2016 US Election: Can democracy survive the internet? Journal of Democracy, 28(2), 63-76.

Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Solso, R. (2004). Attention and cognitive control. Cognitive Psychology: KeyRreadings, 205, 55-85.

Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123-205.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60214-2

Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2011). The elaboration likelihood model. Handbook of theories of social psychology, 1, 224-245.

Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66.

Schneider, W. (1985). Toward a model of attention and the development of automatic processing. In M. I. Posner & O. S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 475-492). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schneider, W., & Chein, J. M. (2003). Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 525-559.

Schneider, W. (2016). Toward a model of attention and the development of automatic processing. In Attention and performance XI (pp. 475-492). Routledge.

Shahin, S., Saldaña, M., & Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2021). Peripheral elaboration model: The impact of incidental news exposure on political participation. Journal of Information Technology & Politics, 18(2), 148–163.

Taylor, M. A., Redfield, J., & Abrams, R. (1981). Neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia and affective disease. Biological Psychiatry, 16, 467–478.

Tucker, J. A., Guess, A., Barberá, P., Vaccari, C., Siegel, A., Sanovich, S., … & Nyhan, B. (2018). Social media, political polarization, and political disinformation: A review of the scientific literature. Political polarization, and political disinformation: a review of the scientific literature (March 19, 2018).

Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2006). Automatic and effortful processing of self‐statements in depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35(2), 117-124.

Weingartner, H., Cohen, R. M., Murphy, D. L., Martello, J., & Gerdt, C. (1981). Cognitive processes in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(1), 42–47.

Website |  + posts

Dr. Cornell has worked in education for more than 20 years. His work has involved designing teacher certification for Trinity College in London and in-service training for state governments in the United States. He has trained kindergarten teachers in 8 countries and helped businessmen and women open baby centers and kindergartens in 3 countries.

This article was peer-reviewed and edited by Chris Drew (PhD). The review process on Helpful Professor involves having a PhD level expert fact check, edit, and contribute to articles. Reviewers ensure all content reflects expert academic consensus and is backed up with reference to academic studies. Dr. Drew has published over 20 academic articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education and holds a PhD in Education from ACU.

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *